Method for Constructing the Natural Scale of Pure Color

From Wikisource
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Method for Constructing the Natural Scale of Pure Color (1909)
by P.G. Nutting
4431034Method for Constructing the Natural Scale of Pure Color1909P.G. Nutting

DEPARTMENT OF COMMERCE AND LABOR


Bulletin

OF THE

Bureau of Standards

S. W. STRATTON, Director


Volume 6

1909-10

[Seal omitted]

WASHINGTON

GOVERNMENT PRINTING OFFICE

1910

A METHOD FOR CONSTRUCTING THE NATURAL SCALE OF PURE COLOR.


By P. G. Nutting.


The color sensation is known to vary far from uniformly with the wave length of the exciting radiation. In a normal spectrum the variation is much more rapid in the yellowish-orange and bluish-green regions than in the midgreen or in the extreme red and violet. Hence, a color scale of say one unit for each 10 μμ difference in wave length would represent far from equal color steps. The difference in wave length just perceptible as a difference in color is roughly 5 μμ in the two most sensitive regions, 15 μμ in the midgreen, and much greater in the violet and red. The method here described makes use of data on this difference limen.

Given the least perceptible difference (difference limen) for an eye throughout the visible spectrum, the reciprocal will be proportional to color sensibility as a function of wave length. But sensibility is the derivative[1] of a scale-reading curve, in this case the color scale desired. A method based on this principle will be applied to some of the best recent data on difference limen to illustrate the construction of a color scale.

Steindler[2] has recently published data on the difference limen of twelve subjects having normal color vision. The characteristics of the color limen curve are shown in the figure. It has two deep minima at about 490 μμ and 580 μμ, a maximum in the green at 535 and a slight maximum and minimum at either end. The location and heights of these seven maxima and minima are given for each of the twelve subjects in the following table:

First
Minima
First
Maxima
Second
Minima
Second
Maxima
Third
Minima
Third
Maxima
Fourth
Minima
λ δλ λ δλ λ δλ λ δλ λ δλ λ δλ
Dr. O.St 435 23.6 454 37.6 495 11.6 535 32.8 585 7.6 626 40.0 638 31.0
Dr. E 434 25.7 450 38.0 488 11.0 532 36.4 587 8.0 630 47.0 651 34.5
Prof. E 430 14.5 444 18.0 480 5.0 523 34.2 586 9.0 624 34.2 637 28.6
Dr. Sch 435 16.0 462 21.6 498 12.0 535 20.4 582 12.4 612 26.0 628 24.0
Dr. A. St 446 26.0 465 34.0 494 25.5 540 49.5 585 22.4 620 38.0 637 32.0
Dr Ma 488 15.9 520 22.0 572 9.1 622 24.0 638 17.2
Dr.Me 436 13.8 448 24.2 478 5.2 545 37.2 598 11.2 630 36.0 646 20.0
Dr. Bi 447 18.0 462 24.0 505 12.0 540 22.2 583 11.2 608 21.2 614 19.2
Hr. B 454 30.4 462 35.5 492 19.6 530 46.0 568 24.0 605 44.0 618 43.0
Frl. M 442 14.0 468 24.4 490 16.0 540 32.0 572 20.0 633 60.0 642 45.5
Dr H 450 51.0 460 54.6 497 7.6 530 46.0 583 19.2
Dr. G 437 39.3 435 39.3 501 22.4 536 22.1 571 12.8 625 42.4 640 34.6
Means 440 24.7 455 29.3 492 13.6 534 33.4 581 13.9 621 37.5 635 30.0


An image should appear at this position in the text.
Fig. 1.

While variations from the mean positions of the maxima and minima are not excessive they are so large that a simple average of all the curves would be smoother than any of them. Adjusting the positions of the maxima and minima of each curve to the means, the following data were obtained for the properties of average (of these twelve) eyes.

TABLE II.

Wave Length (μμ) Color Limen (μμ) Sensibility Ordinate Area Color Scale
420 41.0 244 5.45 21.96
425 36.0 278
430 30.0 333 7.51 21.41
435 26.1 384
440 24.7 405 7.55 20.66
445 26.3 380
450 28.5 351 6.90 19.91
455 29.3 341
460 28.8 347 8.00 19.22
465 24.6 407
470 20.6 485 11.60 18.42
475 17.0 588
480 15.2 13.12 17.26
485 14.0 714
490 13.7 14.44 15.95
495 13.7 730
500 14.5 690 12.04 14.50
505 16.5 607
510 19.4 515 8.70 13.30
515 22.9 436
520 27.4 365 6.56 12.43
525 30.9 324
530 32.7 306 6.04 11.77
535 33.4 300
540 32.5 308 6.60 11.17
545 30.3 330
550 27.1 369 8.80 10.51
555 22.7 440
560 19.2 520 11.72 9.63
565 17.1 588
570 15.4 650 13.90 8.46
575 14.3 700
580 13.9 720 14.00 7.07
585 14.1 709
590 15.2 658 11.36 5.67
595 17.3 578
600 22.2 450 7.32 4.53
605 28.2 355
610 22.8 305 5.58 3.80
615 35.6 281
620 37.5 267 5.48 3.24
625 36.9 271
630 33.5 298 6.48 2.69
635 30.1 332
640 30.3 330 6.24 2.04
645 32.0 313
650 35.6 281 5.21 1.42
655 38.6 259
660 9.00 0.90

The ordinates of the curve of color as a function of wave length are given in the last column. They were obtained by integration of the sensibility curve, the partial areas (divided by two on the scale of the figure) being given in the fourth column. Bach number in the last column is the sum (divided by lo) of the ordinate areas of the fourth column added from the bottom to and including that wave length. The color-wave length curve is plotted in the figure.

A difference of one unit in the color scale represents a difference in color that is just easily perceptible, hence forms a convenient natural unit, although any other subdivision might be used. In Fig. I , each unit of the color scale is indicated on the wave-length axis and just above are indicated roughly the positions of six spectral hues.

To test the theoretical color curve, a normal spectrum was projected on a black screen in which had been cut slits spaced according to the wave lengths of the color units, the slits being covered with ground glass. No departure from uniformity in the color steps could be detected by the ten or more individuals who carefully examined them.

The wave lengths of each of these color steps is given in Table III.

These computations have been carried through merely to illustrate the method. They may easily be made for any eye for which the sensibility curve is known.

If the sensibility curves of a large number of subjects were known, the properties of an average normal human eye might be deduced and a scale of color constructed and adopted.

TABLE III.

Color Wave Length
1 420 Violet
2 435
3 449
4 463 Blue
5 474
6 483
7 490
8 497
9 504
10 514
11 527 Green
12 543
13 556
14 566
15 574
16 580 Yellow
17 588
18 595
19 606 Orange
20 626
21 641
22 658 Red

There is a widespread demand for reference standards of color in terms of which other colors may be specified. Such standards may easily be prepared of any desired hue or shade, but the great difficulties are in choosing rational and uniform divisions on the one hand and in obtaining dyes and pigments that are permanent on the other. Both difficulties would be largely obviated by the adoption of a fixed rational chromatic scale for use as a primary standard.

Washington, April 27, 1909.

2192—No. I—09——7

This work is in the public domain in the United States because it was published before January 1, 1929.


This work may be in the public domain in countries and areas with longer native copyright terms that apply the rule of the shorter term to foreign works.

Public domainPublic domainfalsefalse

  1. This Bulletin 5, p. 266, 1908.
  2. Sitz. Ak. Wiss. Wien, 115, IIa; Jan., 1906.